|
American Institute for Technology
& Science Education Newsletter
|
August, 2012
| |
Greetings!
Did you do it? Did you check out the new, improved AITSE website? If not, be sure you do so this month. It will be worth your time. For a detailed guide through the parts of the site, see the August newsletter in the AITSE archives.
Also, please note that we have updated our bunk-detecting principles. The more analyses of possible bunk we do, the more ways to detect it we pinpoint. We are now up to ten. After you review these, you won't want to miss taking the AITSE bunk-detecting quiz or, if you are between 13 and 19, entering the bunk-detecting essay contest. Spread the news about the principles to your friends (note the share buttons) and about the contest to your children, grandchildren, science teachers, home schoolers, youth groups, etc.
And now, get yourself a healthy snack and settle down to read all about hyperbaric oxygen treatment, alternative medicine, conservation of information, definitions of science, and our upcoming events.
Then, if you enjoy or benefit from our newsletter and work, please seriously consider partnering with us. We cannot continue, and certainly cannot expand, our current work without funds. Unfortunately, these are in very short supply. We currently reach over 168,000 people through our newsletter, Facebook sites, Linkedin updates, and tweets. This is not even considering those who are impacted through our radio interviews and speaking engagements. But, we would like to be able to expand our work. For example, we would like to pursue providing a regular bunk-detecting radio program, having an annual conference, publishing bunk-free curriculum and a course on integrity in science, and bringing significant speakers to various parts of the country. Therefore, allow me to challenge you to commit to a regular donation to AITSE--just $20/month from 100 people would go a long way in advancing and developing our programs.
|
|
What is Science?
Introduction
How many 'sciences' are there? Science is thought by many to be the most global-universal thing we've got available to humanity nowadays, other than perhaps football (soccer) teams and the United Nations. It is supposed to be neutral to gender, race, ethnicity, class, network, status, ideology, political system and religion. Since it is nowadays generally held that there is more than one science, i.e. that science is plural, not singular, that there are multiple scientific methods and not just a single, uniform scientific method, the text below is my attempt to answer the above question by giving a basic guide for how to estimate the approximate number of sciences.
As orientation lessons on the history and philosophy of science (HPS) often begin, there are two questions we must ask: which science(s) and whose science(s)? The first question is mainly what I am focused on in this paper. But the second question is likewise important because people hold various opinions about what constitutes 'science' and what doesn't. To some scientists, other scientists don't actually count as 'scientists' because they are considered not scientific enough, i.e. their field is not really a 'scientific' field according to others' perception.
It would thus be impossible for me to give a definitive answer here about exactly how many sciences there are from an objective perspective. Instead what I propose to do is to look at what several people and organizations consider to be 'science,' to ask a few questions about their views and then to offer my own interpretation near the end, including a brief summary. This way, the reader won't think I'm imposing my personal way of defining science as authoritative compared with their own. For some brief background context, the way I will approach the question of how many sciences there are is in light of training and work done on the topic of HPS, science studies and several sociology of science traditions.
Organizing and Categorizing the Sciences
Let us start at a globally-widespread institution that believes in the unity of knowledge still today and in the concept of 'unity-in-diversity,' i.e. the foundational meaning of the 'university.' The Catholic Church contributes to science through its Pontifical Academy of Sciences and names 9 academies distinguished as follows: 1. Astronomy, 2. Chemistry, 3. Earth and Environment Sciences, 4. Life Sciences (which includes 4.1 Botany, 4.2 Agronomy, 4.3 Zoology, 4.4 Genetics, 4.5 Molecular Biology, 4.6 Biochemistry, 4.7 Neuroscience, 4.8 Surgery), 5. Mathematics, 6. Application of Science, 7. Philosophy and History of Science (Epistemology), 8. Physics, 9. Other Disciplines. What I find most interesting in the list is the inclusion of "Application(s) of Science" as a separate scientific branch of knowledge, as well as multiple fields being classified under 'Life Sciences.'
We can also look to those who study science scientifically. The field known as 'science studies' (or naukovedeniye in its original Russian), offers us 'scientific' insights into what qualifies as scientific fields. In some recent cases, this has been visually 'mapped' according to various branches of knowledge . The Knowledge Mapping Laboratory of the University of California, San Diego identifies 13 main branches. Here it must be noted that in the Anglo-American tradition, 'humanities' does not count as a 'scientific' field, whereas in the German-Russian tradition, there is a 'science of humanities' or 'humanitarian science.' In any case, we can identify several main types or branches of science, similar but different from the above example, some of which can be grouped under joint or collaborative labels.
Cynthia F. Kirkeby runs a website called ClassBrain.com which claims to be "tailored to the informational needs and skill levels of the various age groups ." The site speaks of 10 Kinds of Scientists (2006). Basically, scientists are those who study the fields denoted by their professional names; astronomers study astronomy, biologists study biology, chemists study chemistry, and so on.
Looking at a more common, less academic source to see what the so-called average educated person might consider to be 'science,' Wiki Answers responds to the question: "How many types of science are there ?" It claims there are 30 scientific fields. This list defines as independent scientific fields some 'sciences' that in Kirkeby's model and in the above approaches would instead be considered as scientific subfields, rather than autonomous disciplines.
But now that we have reached a view of 30 sciences, can this be said to be an exhaustive list? Could there possibly be more than 30 kinds of science? One source identifies 612 branches of science. Should we grant that all of the fields listed at Wiki Answers are 'scientific,' including Ufology? Or what about SETI (the search for extra-terrestrial intelligence) - is that properly considered as a scientific field? What about 'origins of life' (OoL) - is that a scientific field, or rather more closely related to philosophy or even theology or worldview studies? What are the proper grounds for integration and overlap? If some academic fields are best not called 'sciences,' then what qualifies us to reject certain fields as scientific while accepting others?
A Social-Philosophical View of Science
The so-called 'science demarcation game' was part of the 'Science Wars ' of the 1990's and 2000's, brought on by natural scientists questioning the scientificity of social sciences and literary studies. The latter fields had been engaging in an on-going analysis of scientists and scientific practice, including the subfield of sociology of science (SoS), which followed natural-physical scientists around to see what they were actually doing and engaged in interviews and dialogues with them, thus in a sense 'demystifying' scientific behavior and attitudes.
Likewise, SoS for decades had been studying the personal and collective meanings of science and scientific knowledge to people; how the public, how civil society, how individuals view science and its value to humanity. This inevitably led to some challenges to the positivistic, pseudo-neutral and objectivistic approaches to science that are still to be found in some philosophies of science. It thus put scientists on the defensive regarding their legitimacy, their credibility and their appropriate value to society and a reaction was to be expected.
The 'Science Wars,' along with HPS, science studies and SoS have informed the background for this survey about how many sciences there are and what distinguishes them from non-sciences. Personally, I have found it helpful to distinguish roughly 10 kinds of sciences and 8 categories of science (which can be found here), based on my academic work in these 3 fields. But as I said above, it is likely that readers will desire to have their own chosen number of sciences as they designate and assign knowledge fields into their personal 'map of science.'
Here is one basic example: An apparent rift between creationism, Intelligent Design and evolution in debates about origins and processes of change-over-time is also sometimes defined as that between 'operations sciences' and 'origins sciences.' Whereas origins sciences analyze an object or topic after-the-fact or try to reverse-engineer it to understand or explain events that happened in the remote past, operations sciences deal with operations or experiments that are or can be done in the present. This origins science vs. operations science dichotomy, however, has been widely questioned by many critics of ID and of creationism and may be also categorized under different names. Does the reader embrace origins science vs. operations science or reject it as a false dichotomy?
Let me also add a short word of caution regarding the term 'historical sciences,' which is a debatable category. I am not entirely convinced that the term 'historical sciences' makes much sense or whether other alternatives, such as chronological sciences, geographical sciences, musical sciences or worldview sciences should be added alongside it for balance. Regardless of my hesitancy, however, many people consider history as a kind of science, in addition to being a unique category involving certain methods and theories. Let me therefore withhold judgment about whether or not 'sciences of history' or 'historical sciences' are appropriate terms of use.
Conclusion
There are of course many other ways that people have identified and categorized the various sciences and fields of knowledge than those presented here (e.g. the Dewey Decimal System or the Chinese Library Classification). The aim of this paper has been to present a few of the options that are currently available for organizing and categorizing scientific knowledge and for identifying the limits of science. It is hoped that this principle concern will provoke readers to conceptualize and to seek to imagine their own 'map of science' for the purpose of exploring the 'unity in diversity' of knowledge in the electronic-information age.
Read more.
|
Come and Hear! AITSE Presentations
AITSE members will be meeting at the Cannery in Newport Beach on October 5 at 6 pm. Come, meeting the others who are concerned about integrity in science, and hear all about what AITSE is doing. For $50, its cheap at the price!
Dr. Caroline Crocker will be giving a presentation on the AITSE Bunk-Detecting Principles at Logos Building, Costa Mesa, CA at 7 pm on October 6. If you are in the area, come-- and hone your bunk-detecting skills.
|
|
Hyperbaric Oxygen Treatment
Hope or Hoax?
A revolutionary new treatment for diabetes, asthma, autism, PTSD, cerebral palsy, AIDS, heart disease, aging, learning disabilities, Alzheimer's, arthritis, alcoholism, multiple sclerosis, hair loss, impotence and more has come to AITSE's attention. Apparently, it can even restore sight to a person born blind due to birth injuries (p. 92)! The book about this treatment, The Oxygen Revolution, by Paul Harch MD and Virginia McCullough sounds like it is presenting miracles for the asking. But is it? Or is this book rather a great example of bunk science? Not because hyperbaric oxygen therapy (HBOT) itself is bunk; it is not when used in the right circumstances. But, because using HBOT (high pressure oxygen therapy) beyond its proven uses without valid scientific reasoning and before such use has been clinically evaluated in controlled peer-reviewed studies is bunk.
Therefore, the objections to this book are not because, as Harch claims (p. xxiv), AITSE is letting others do our thinking for us. Rather this article is based on a combination of 1) an analysis of The Oxygen Revolution using the AITSE bunk-detecting principles (it fails every one), 2) a review of the scientific literature on HBOT and traumatic brain injury (TBI) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in particular, and 3) the input of various experts.
AITSE Bunk-Detection
1. Check if the author claims that something has been proven or declares something to be a fact.
Although the book does not use the words "fact" or "proven," declaring that "we've discovered that" HBOT "permanently" changes "the body's tissues" comes uncomfortably close (p. 4), especially since nothing about this has been published in the peer-reviewed literature.
Another chapter speaks about a single patient with traumatic brain injury (TBI) who, after HBOT, experienced a 40% improvement in his computational abilities (p. 65). This "huge change" was acclaimed as being "highly statistically significant" and "unquestionably not a chance event." This claim implies that statistical tests have been run and that they have shown HBOT to have been what made the difference. But note that three significant errors are being made. The first two have to do with statistics and the third with the interpretation of the data. It is the nature of statistics that they calculate the likelihood of something occurring by chance--that is, the probability of it occurring outside of the normal bell-shaped curve. To calculate this one requires that the "n" or number of trials is greater than "1." But, this report is about one incident in the life of one person only. Then, there are issues with the understanding of what a statistical test can show. Although a probability may be small (unlikely with n=1), it is never "0." The words "unquestionably not...chance," imply it is. Finally, it is being assumed that, because the patient received HBOT treatments, his improvement was due to the HBOT. But, correlation does not mean causation. After all, it is well documented that people do heal from TBI--and this entirely because of natural processes.
Moreover, claiming that something is "unquestionable" is not a scientific statement. Scientists are trained to be accurate, be skeptical and consider the options. Principle 1: FAILED
2. Check if the author makes claims to have accomplished something that is beyond what has actually been done or is even possible to do. Read more
|
Quotes of the Month
By Stephen Barrett, MD
"Homeopathic "remedies" are usually harmless, but their associated misbeliefs are not. When people are healthy, it may not matter what they believe. But when serious illness strikes, false beliefs can lead to disaster."
"Many naturopathic theories and practices are not based on the body of basic knowledge related to health, disease, and health care which has been widely accepted by the scientific community."
"Chelation therapy...is falsely claimed to be effective against cardiovascular disease, autism, and many other diseases and conditions. Because chelation has valid use in some cases of heavy metal poisoning, many practitioners falsely diagnose lead, mercury, or other heavy metal toxicity to trick patients into undergoing chelation."
These and many other tidbits about alternative medicine are offered at Quackwatch, a website operated by Dr. Stephen Barrett. Here one is advised to base medical decisions on good science, based on evidence, not anecdotes or unscientific ideologies.
AITSE's goal is similar to Dr. Barrett's. It is to help the public discern between bunk and real science. Be sure to read about the bunk detecting principles and the host of articles we offer on alternative medicine--you'll be glad you did.
|
Conservation of Information Made Simple
by William Dembski, PhD
This article by AITSE Consortium member William Dembski was first published at Evolution News and Views. It is re-posted with his permission.
In the 1970s, Doubleday published a series of books with the title "Made Simple." This series covered a variety of academic topics (*Statistics Made Simple*, *Philosophy Made Simple*, etc.). The 1980s saw the "For Dummies" series, which expanded the range of topics to include practical matters such as auto repair. The "For Dummies" series has since been replicated, notably by guides for "Complete Idiots." All books in these series attempt, with varying degrees of success, to break down complex subjects, helping students to learn a topic, especially when they've been stymied by more conventional approaches and textbooks.
In this article, I'm going to follow the example of these books, laying out as simply and clearly as I can what conservation of information is and why it poses a challenge to conventional evolutionary thinking. I'll break this concept down so that it seems natural and straightforward. Right now, it's too easy for critics of intelligent design to say, "Oh, that conservation of information stuff is just mumbo-jumbo. It's part of the ID agenda to make a gullible public think there's some science backing ID when it's really all smoke and mirrors." Conservation of information is not a difficult concept and once it is understood, it becomes clear that evolutionary processes cannot create the information required to power biological evolution.
*Conservation of Information: A Brief History*
Conservation of information is a term with a short history. Biologist Peter Medawar used it in the 1980s to refer to mathematical and computational systems that are limited to producing logical consequences from a given set of axioms or starting points, and thus can create no novel information (everything in the consequences is already implicit in the starting points). His use of the term is the first that I know, though the idea he captured with it is much older. Note that he called it the "Law of Conservation of Information" (see his *The Limits of Science*, 1984).
Computer scientist Tom English, in a 1996 paper, also used the term conservation of information, though synonymously with the then recently proved results by Wolpert and Macready about No Free Lunch (NFL). In English's version of NFL, "the information an optimizer gains about unobserved values is ultimately due to its prior information of value distributions." As with Medawar's form of conservation of information, information for English is not created from scratch but rather redistributed from existing sources.
Conservation of information, as the idea is being developed and gaining currency in the intelligent design community, is principally the work of Bob Marks and myself, along with several of Bob's students at Baylor. Conservation of information, as we use the term, applies to search. Now search may seem like a fairly
restricted topic. Unlike conservation of energy, which applies at all scales and dimensions of the universe, conservation of information, in focusing on search, may seem to have only limited physical significance. But in fact, conservation of information is deeply embedded in the fabric of nature, and the term does not misrepresent its own importance. Read more.
|
|
|
|
In closing, as always, thank you for your past gifts and support. It is a fact that AITSE cannot function in its efforts to educate to increase scientific understanding and integrity without contributions. Please consider helping us with a special donation or a commitment to give on a monthly basis. Please make checks payable to AITSE and send them to PO Box 15938, Newport Beach, CA 92659. Alternatively, you can donate on line through PayPal or credit card.
Sincerely,  Caroline Crocker
American Institute for Technology and Science Education |
|
|
|